συντακτικό.pdf

download συντακτικό.pdf

of 11

description

συντακτικο

Transcript of συντακτικό.pdf

  • In: Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Department of Linguistics, School of Philology, Faculty of Philosophy, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, May 23, 2009. Thessaloniki: Institute of Modern Greek Studies [Institute Manoli Triantafillidi], 477-486.

    1 2

    1 1

    Abstract

    This paper investigates how L1 and L2 children process garden-path sentences, such as:

    (1) While she was eating the pizza fell on the floor and (2) While she was sleeping the

    pizza fell on the floor. Garden-path effects have been claimed to results from the parsers preference for the structurally simplest analysis. Garden-path effects do not arise with

    intransitive verbs, such as in (2), because readers make use of subcategorisation

    information of the verb. In English, Traxler (2002), using a self-paced reading task

    showed that when punctuation was not present L1 English 8-to-12 year-old children are

    garden-pathed. To date studies in the processing of garden-path sentences in Greek L1

    children have failed to show garden-path effects (Papadopoulou & Tsimpli, 2005).

    However, this study used the moving-window paradigm and involved reading.

    Childrens developing literacy may have interfered in processing causing the lack of garden-path effects. The present study has used a self-paced listening task manipulating

    argument structure. Given that intonation provides cues for clause boundaries,

    Experiment 1 used flat intonation to eliminate those cues; Experiment 2 used natural

    intonation to examine whether intonation cues reduce garden-path effects. 22 9;6-to-12;5

    year-old Russian-Greek children and 22 9;0-to-12;1 monolingual Greek children took

    part in the two experiments. Analyses of accuracy showed that L2 children were less

    accurate than L1 children in both experiments. Natural prosody facilitated sentence

    processing in L1 but not L2 children. In the absence of prosodic cues (Exp. 1) L1

    children showed garden-path effects. L2 children, on the other hand, were not garden-

    pathed. When natural intonation provided cues for sentence boundaries, there was no

    garden-path effect for any of the groups. This shows that when tested in the auditory

    modality, L2 children do not process garden-path sentences as L1 children; they are not

    sensitive to subcategorisation information of the verb or to prosodic cues. The L2

    childrens absence of sensitivity to these two features can lead to comprehension difficulties which may affect their school performance and overall educational

    attainment.

    1 Reading

  • 2

    1

    (1) (2) . (1) (1) ().

    (1) While she was eating the pizza fell on the floor ( /- ) (1) While she was eating, the pizza fell on the floor ( , )

    (1) the pizza (eat) (Frazier & Fodor, 1978). (fall). . /- garden-path effect (late closure principle). (1). , (1), . (2) (2) :

    (2) While she was sleeping the pizza fell on the floor. ( /- ) (2) While she was sleeping, the pizza fell on the floor. ( , )

    (sleep), . . , , (Marslen-Wilson, Tyler, Warren, Grenier, & Lee, 1992; Nagel, Shapiro, Tuller, & Nawy, 1996).

  • 3

    , . . . , Traxler (2002) (1) (2) 8 12 . . , . , . . .

    , . , Papadopoulou & Tsimpli (2005) 10-11 . 4 . . . .

    . . , (self-paced listening task) . 6 (Booth, Mac Whinney, & Harasaki, 2000; Felser, Marinis, & Clahsen, 2003). , . (Marslen-Wilson et al., 1992; Nagel et al., 1996). . 5

  • 4

    (Beach, Katz, & Skowronski, 1996; Wells, Peppe, & Goulandris, 2004), (Choi & Mazuka, 2003; Snedeker & Trueswell, 2003). .

    (1: ) (1: , 2: ) . : 1 2 .

    2 2.1

    1 , (Stavrakaki & Tsimpli, 2000) , (, ).

    (%)

    ()

    1

    10.5

    (127)

    27

    (89%)

    15

    (89%)

    17

    (95%)

    59

    (91%)

    - 2

    11.1

    (134)

    24

    (79%)

    13

    (79%)

    16

    (88%)

    53

    (81%)

    1.

    2 (on-line) . (t (40) = -2.049, p = .047), . 1 2 (t (40) = 3.973, p = .000), (t (40) = 3.237, p = .002) (t (40) = 3.536, p = .001).

    2.2

    88 : 8 -, 40 40 (fillers).

  • 5

    4 . . 2 : ( , ) (, ) 4 . (3)-(6).

    (3) - (): / / / (4) - (): / / / (5) - () * / / / (6) - () / / /

    5 5 , 2 . 1 , . 2 .

    2.3

    . . . E-Prime(Schneider, Eschmann, & Zuccolotto, 2002).

    2.4

    . ANOVA 2X2X2

    : (1 , 2 ), (, ) (, ).

  • 6

    3 3.1 1 ( ):

    ( 1) 1 ( ).

    1: ( ms) 1 : : / / / : / / / *: / / / : / / /

    1 ( 3) ( 4). 3 (F1 (1, 19) = 7.512, p= .013, F2 (1, 6) = 7.417, p= .034), . - () (): t (19) = -2.470, p = .023, 1 . 2 3.

    4, 1 (, ) (F1 (1, 19) = 6.397, p= .020, F2 (1, 6) = 4.984, p= .067). ( < : t (19) = 2.549, p = .020)

  • 7

    . 2 4 (F1 (1, 21) = 4.931, p= .038, F2 (1, 6) = 3.878, p= .096), 1 .

    3.2 1:

    1 , 1 2 . 1 2 -. 1 (Papadopoulou & Tsimpli, 2005) . 2 . . , (1) . (Traxler, 2002). (2) 1 . , .

    3.3 2 ( ):

    2 2 .

  • 8

    2: (ms) 2 : : / / / : / / / *: / / / : / / /

    ANOVAs 2X2X2 2 . 1 ( 3) (F1 (1, 19) = 5.187, p= .035, F2 (1, 6) = 5.809, p= .053), . 2 .

    3.4 2:

    , . , 1.

    3.5

    2 .

  • 9

    1: 2:

    % (SD) 1 2 1 2

    MA 68 (23.0) 64 (30.1) 84 (16.2) 59 (24.1)

    72 (24.9) 64 (16.0) 78 (18.3) 68 (16.8) * 91 (10.5) 77 (26.1) 87 (20.7) 80 (26.8) 76 (23.2) 72 (19.5) 83 (19.5) 66 (21.8)

    2. .

    ANOVAs 2X2X22 (, ), (, ), (1 , 2) ( , ) (F1 (1, 40) = 3.768, p= .059, F2 (1, 12) = 5.829, p= .033). 1 2 (F1 (1, 19) = 6.771, p= .018, F2 (1, 6) = 5.938, p= .051) 2 .

    4

    , (1) (2) . , 2 1 1 . . 1 . (Papadopoulou & Tsimpli, 2005), . . - , . .

    2 , , -

  • 10

    , / . 2 . , - .

    2 , . 1 ( 4). 2 1 2 . 1, 2 .

    1 9-12 2 . 2 9-12 -. . 2 , .

    Beach, C. M., Katz, W. F., & Skowronski, A. (1996). Children's processing of prosodic

    cues for phrasal interpretation. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 99(2),

    1148-1160.

    Booth, J., Mac Whinney, B., & Harasaki, Y. (2000). Developmental differences in visual

    and auditory processing of complex sentences. Child Development, 71, 981-

    1003.

    Choi, Y., & Mazuka, R. (2003). Young Children's Use of Prosody in Sentence parsing.

    Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32(2), 197-217.

    Felser, C., Marinis, T., & Clahsen, H. (2003). Childrens processing of ambiguous sentences: A study of relative clause attachment. Language Acquisition, 11,

    127-163.

  • 11

    Frazier, L., & Fodor, J. D. (1978). The sausage machine: a new two-stage parsing model.

    Cognition, 6, 291-325.

    Marslen-Wilson, W., Tyler, L., Warren, P., Grenier, P., & Lee, C. S. (1992). Prosodic

    effects in minimal attachment. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,

    45A, 73-87.

    Nagel, H. N., Shapiro, L. P., Tuller, B., & Nawy, R. (1996). Prosodic influences on the

    resolution of temporary ambiguity during on-line sentence processing. Journal

    of Psycholinguistic Research, 25, 319-344.

    Papadopoulou, D., & Tsimpli, I.-M. (2005). Morphological cues in childrens processing of ambiguous sentences: a study of subject/ object ambiguities in Greek. In A.

    Brugos, M. R. Clark-Cotton & S. Ha (Eds.), 29th Annual Boston University

    Conference on Language Development (pp. 471-481). Boston: Cascadilla Press.

    Schneider, W., Eschmann, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime users guide. Pittsburgh: PA: Psychology Software Tools, Inc.

    Snedeker, J., & Trueswell, J. (2003). Using Prosody to Avoid Ambiguity: Effects of

    Speaker Awareness and Referential Context. Journal of Memory and

    Language, 48, 103-130.

    Stavrakaki, S., & Tsimpli, I. (2000). Diagnostic Verbal IQ Test for Greek preschool and

    school age children:standardization, statistical analysis, psychometric

    properties, 8th Symposium of he Panhellenic Association of Logopedists (pp.

    95-106). Athens: Ellinika Grammata.

    Traxler, M. (2002). Plausibility and subcategorization preference in childrens processing of temporarily ambiguous sentences: Evidence from self-paced

    reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 75-96.

    Wells, B., Peppe, S., & Goulandris, N. (2004). Intonation development from five to

    thirteen. Journal of Child Language(31), 749-778.